For other uses, see Folklore (disambiguation).

Folklore is the body of expressive culture shared by a particular group of people; it encompasses the traditions common to that culture, subculture or group. These include oral traditions such as tales, proverbs and jokes. They include material culture, ranging from traditional building styles to handmade toys common to the group. Folklore also includes customary lore, the forms and rituals of celebrations like Christmas and weddings, folk dances and initiation rites. Each one of these, either singly or in combination, is considered a folklore artifact. Just as essential as the form, folklore also encompasses the transmission of these artifacts from one region to another or from one generation to the next. For folklore is not taught in a formal school curriculum or studied in the fine arts. Instead these traditions are passed along informally from one individual to another either through verbal instruction or demonstration. The academic study of folklore is called folkloristics.

Netherlandish Proverbs

To fully understand folklore, it is helpful to clarify its component parts: the terms folk and lore. It is well-documented that the term was coined in 1846 by the Englishman William Thoms. He fabricated it to replace the contemporary terminology of "popular antiquities" or "popular literature". The second half of the compound word, lore, proves easier to define as its meaning has stayed relatively stable over the last two centuries. Coming from Old English lār ‘instruction,’ and with German and Dutch cognates, it is the knowledge and traditions of a particular group, frequently passed along by word of mouth.[1]

The concept of folk proves somewhat more elusive. When Thoms first created this term, folk applied only to rural, frequently poor, frequently illiterate peasants. A more modern definition of folk is a social group which includes two or more persons with common traits, who express their shared identity through distinctive traditions. "Folk is a flexible concept which can refer to a nation as in American folklore or to a single family."[2] This expanded social definition of folk supports a broader view of the material, i.e. the lore, considered to be folklore artifacts. These now include all "things people make with words (verbal lore), things they make with their hands (material lore), and things they make with their actions (customary lore)".[3] Folklore is no longer circumscribed as being chronologically old or obsolete. The folklorist studies the traditional artifacts of a social group and how they are transmitted.

Transmission is a vital part of the folklore process. Without communicating these beliefs and customs within the group over space and time, they would become cultural shards relegated to cultural archaeologists. For folklore is also a verb. These folk artifacts continue to be passed along informally, as a rule anonymously and always in multiple variants. The folk group is not individualistic, it is community-based and nurtures its lore in community. “As new groups emerge, new folklore is created… surfers, motorcyclists, computer programmers”.[4] In direct contrast to high culture, where any single work of a named artist is protected by copyright law, folklore is a function of shared identity within the social group.[5]

Having identified folk artifacts, the professional folklorist strives to understand the significance of these beliefs, customs and objects for the group. For these cultural units[6] would not be passed along unless they had some continued relevance within the group. That meaning can however shift and morph. So Halloween of the 21st century is not the All Hallows’ Eve of the Middle Ages, and even gives rise to its own set of urban legends independent of the historical celebration. The cleansing rituals of Orthodox Judaism were originally good public health in a land with little water; now these customs signify identification as an Orthodox Jew. Compare this to brushing your teeth, also transmitted within a group, which remains a practical hygiene and health issue and does not rise to the level of a group-defining tradition.[7] For tradition is initially remembered behavior. Once it loses its practical purpose, there is no reason for further transmission unless it has been imbued with meaning beyond the initial practicality of the action. This meaning is at the core of folkloristics, the study of folklore.

With an increasingly theoretical sophistication of the social sciences, it has become evident that folklore is a naturally occurring and necessary component of any social group, it is indeed all around us.[8] It does not have to be old or antiquated. It continues to be created, transmitted and in any group is used to differentiate between "us" and "them".


Origin and development of Folklore Studies

Folklore began to distinguish itself as an autonomous discipline during the period of romantic nationalism in Europe. A particular figure in this development was Johann Gottfried von Herder, whose writings in the 1770s presented oral traditions as organic processes grounded in locale. After the German states were invaded by Napoleonic France, Herder's approach was adopted by many of his fellow Germans who systematized the recorded folk traditions and used them in their process of nation building. This process was enthusiastically embraced by smaller nations like Finland, Estonia, and Hungary, which were seeking political independence from their dominant neighbours.[9]

Folklore as a field of study further developed among 19th century European scholars who were contrasting tradition with the newly developing modernity. Its focus was the oral folklore of the rural peasant populations, which were considered as residue and survivals of the past that continued to exist within the lower strata of society.[10] The "Kinder- und Hausmärchen" of the Brothers Grimm (first published 1812) is the best known but by no means only collection of verbal folklore of the European peasantry of that time. This interest in stories, sayings and songs continued throughout the 19th century and aligned the fledgling discipline of fol-kloristics with literature and mythology. By the turn into the 20th century the number and sophistication of folklore studies and folklorists had grown both in Europe and North America. Whereas European folklorists remained focused on the oral folklore of the homogenous peasant populations in their regions, the American folklorists, led by Franz Boas and Ruth Benedict, chose to consider Native American cultures in their research, and included the totality of their customs and beliefs as folklore. This distinction aligned American folkloristics with cultural anthropology and ethnology, using the same techniques of data collection in their field research. This divided alliance of folkloristics between the humanities in Europe and the social sciences in America offers a wealth of theoretical vantage points and research tools to the field of folkloristics as a whole, even as it continues to be a point of discussion within the field itself.[11]

The term Folkloristics, along with its synonym Folklore Studies,[note 1] gained currency in the 1950s to distinguish the academic study of traditional culture from the folklore artifacts themselves. With the passage in 1976 of the American Folklife Preservation Act, (P.L. 94-201),[12] passed by the U.S. Congress in conjunction with the Bicentennial Celebration in 1976, folkloristics in the United States came of age.

"…[Folklife] means the traditional expressive culture shared within the various groups in the United States: familial, ethnic, occupational, religious, regional; expressive culture includes a wide range of creative and symbolic forms such as custom, belief, technical skill, language, literature, art, architecture, music, play, dance, drama, ritual, pageantry, handicraft; these expressions are mainly learned orally, by imitation, or in performance, and are generally maintained without benefit of formal instruction or institutional direction."

Added to the panoply of other legislation designed to protect the natural and cultural heritage of the United States, this law also marks a shift in our national awareness. It gives voice to a growing understanding that the cultural diversity of the United States is a national strength and a resource worthy of protection. Paradoxically, it is a unifying feature, not something that separates us. "We no longer view cultural difference as a problem to be solved, but as a tremendous opportunity. In the diversity of American folklife we find a marketplace teeming with the exchange of traditional forms and cultural ideas, a rich resource for Americans".[13] This diversity is celebrated annually at the Smithsonian Folklife Festival and many other folklife fests around the country.

Who are the folk?

The folk of the 19th century, the social group identified in the original term “folklore”, was characterized by being rural, non-literate and poor. They were the peasants living in the countryside, in contrast to the urban populace of the cities. Only toward the end of the century did the urban proletariat (on the coattails of Marxist theory) become included with the rural poor as folk. The common feature in this expanded definition of folk was their identification as the underclass of society.[14]

Moving forward into the 20th century, in tandem with new thinking in the social sciences, folklorists also revised and expanded their concept of the folk group. By the 1960’s it was understood that social groups, i.e. folk groups, were all around us; each individual is enmeshed in a multitude of differing identities and their concomitant social groups. The first group that each of us is born into is the family, and each family has its own unique folklore. As a child grows into an individual, its identities also increase to include age, language, ethnicity, occupation, etc. Each of these cohorts has its own folklore, and as one folklorist points out, this is “not idle speculation… Decades of fieldwork have demonstrated conclusively that these groups do have their own folklore.”[15] In this modern understanding, folklore is a function of shared identity within any social group.[16]

This folklore can include jokes, sayings and expected behavior in multiple variants, always transmitted in an informal manner. For the most part it will be learned by observation, imitation, repetition or correction by other group members. This informal knowledge is used to confirm and re-inforce the identity of the group. It can be used both internally within the group to express their common identity, for example in an initiation ceremony for new members. Or it can be used externally to differentiate the group from outsiders, like a folkdance demonstration at a community festival. Significant to folklorists here is that there are two opposing but equally valid ways to use this in the study of a group: you can start with an identified group in order to explore its folklore, or you can identify folklore items and use them to identify the social group.[17]

Beginning in the 1960’s, a further expansion of the concept of folk began to unfold in folkloristics. Individual researchers identified folk groups which had previously been overlooked and ignored. One major example of this is found in an issue of “The Journal of American Folklore”, published 1975. This edition is dedicated exclusively to articles on women’s folklore, with approaches that were not coming from a man’s perspective.[note 2] Other groups that were highlighted as part of this broadened understanding of the folk group were non-traditional families, occupational groups, and families that pursued production of folk items through multiple generations.

Genres: the lore of folklore

Individual folklore artifacts are commonly classified as one of three types: material, verbal or customary lore. For the most part self-explanatory, these categories include physical objects (material folklore), common sayings, expressions, stories and songs (verbal folklore), and beliefs and ways of doing things (customary folklore). There is also a fourth major subgenre defined for children’s folklore and games (childlore), as the collection and interpretation of this fertile topic is peculiar to school yards and neighborhood streets.[18] Each of these genres and their subtypes is intended to organize and categorize the folklore artifacts; they provide common vocabulary and consistent labeling for folklorists to communicate with each other.

That said, each artifact is unique; in fact one of the characteristics of all folklore artifacts is their variation within genres and types.[19] This is in direct contrast to manufactured goods, where the goal in production is to create products which are identical, and variations are considered mistakes. It is however just this required variation that makes identification and classification of the defining features a challenge. And while this classification is essential for the subject area of folkloristics, it remains just labeling, and adds little to an understanding of the traditional development and meaning of the artifacts themselves.[20]

Necessary as they are, genre classifications are misleading in their oversimplification of the subject area. Folklore artifacts are never self-contained, they do not stand in isolation but are particulars in the self-representation of a community. Different genres are frequently combined with each other to mark an event.[21] So a birthday celebration might include a song or formulaic way of greeting the birthday child (verbal), presentation of a cake and wrapped presents (material), as well as customs to honor the individual, such as sitting at the head of the table, and blowing out the candles with a wish. There might also be special games played at birthday parties which are not generally played at other times. Adding to the complexity of the interpretation, the birthday party for a seven-year-old will not be identical to the birthday party for that same child as a six-year-old, even though they follow the same model. For each artifact embodies a single variant of a performance in a given time and space. The task of the folklorist becomes to identify within this surfeit of variables the constants and the expressed meaning that shimmer through all variations: honoring of the individual within the circle of family and friends, gifting to express their value and worth to the group, and of course, the festival food and drink as signifiers of the event.

Verbal Tradition

The formal definition of verbal lore is words, both written and oral, which are “spoken, sung, voiced forms of traditional utterance that show repetitive patterns.”[22] Crucial here are the repetitive patterns. Verbal lore is not just any conversation, but words and phrases conforming to a traditional configuration recognized by both the speaker and the audience. For narrative types by definition have consistent structure, and follow an existing model in their narrative form.[note 3] As just one simple example, in English the phrase “An elephant walks into a bar…” instantaneously flags the following text as a joke. It might be one you’ve already heard, but it might be one that the speaker has just thought up within the current context. This is folklore in action. Another example is the child’s song Old MacDonald Had a Farm, where each performance is distinctive in the animals named, their order and their sounds. Songs such as this are used to express cultural values (farms are important, farmers are old and weather-beaten) and teach children about different domesticated animals.[23] This is folklore in action.

Verbal folklore was the original folklore, the artifacts defined by William Thoms as older, oral cultural traditions of the rural populace. In his 1846 published call for help in documenting antiquities, Thoms was echoing scholars from across the European continent to collect artifacts of verbal lore. By the beginning of the 20th century these collections had grown to include artifacts from around the world and across several centuries. A system to organize and categorize them became necessary.[24] Antti Aarne published a first classification system for folktales in 1910. This was later expanded into the Aarne–Thompson classification system by Stith Thompson and remains the standard classification system for European folktales and other types of oral literature. As the number of classified oral artifacts grew, similarities were noted in items which had been collected from very different geographic regions, ethnic groups and epochs, giving rise to the Historic-Geographic Method, a methodology which dominated folkloristics in the first half of the 20th century.

When William Thoms first published his appeal to document the verbal lore of the rural populations, it was believed these folk artifacts would die out as the population became literate. Over the past two centuries this belief has proven to be wrong; folklorists continue to collect verbal lore in both written and spoken form from all social groups. Some variants might have been captured in published collections, but much of it is still transmitted orally and indeed continues to be generated in new forms and variants at an alarming rate.

The story of Jahangir and Anarkali is popular folklore in the former territories of the Mughal Empire.

Below is listed a small sampling of types and examples of verbal lore.

Material culture

Horse and sulky weathervane, Smithsonian American Art Museum

The genre of material culture includes all artifacts that you can touch, hold, live in or eat. They are tangible objects, with a physical presence intended for use either permanently or just at next meal. Most of these folklore artifacts are single objects which have been created by hand for a specific purpose. However folk artifacts can also be mass produced, such as dreidels or Christmas decorations. These items continue to be considered folklore due to their long (pre-industrial) history and their customary use. All of these material objects “existed prior to and continue alongside mechanized industry. … [They are] transmitted across the generations and subject to the same forces of conservative tradition and individual variation”[25] that are found in all folk artifacts. Of interest to folklorists are their physical form, their method of manufacture or construction, their pattern of use as well as the procurement of the raw materials.[26] The meaning to those who both make and use these objects is important. Of primary significance in these studies is the complex balance of continuity over change in both their design and their decoration.

Traditional highlanders' pins hand-made by a goldsmith in Podhale, Poland

In Europe before the Industrial Revolution everything was made by hand. While some folklorists of the 19th century wanted to secure the oral traditions of the rural folk before the populace became literate, other folklorists sought to identify hand-crafted objects before their production processes were lost to industrial manufacturing. Just as verbal lore continues to be actively created and transmitted in today’s culture, so these handicrafts, possibly with a shift in purpose and meaning, can still be found all around us. For there are many reasons to continue to hand make objects for use. It could mean these skills are needed to repair manufactured items. Or perhaps a unique design is wanted which is not (or cannot be) found in the stores. Many crafts are considered to be simple home maintenance, such as cooking, sewing and carpentry. Handicrafts have also become for many an enjoyable and satisfying hobby. Last but not least, handmade objects have taken on the sheen of prestige, where extra time and thought is spent in their creation and their uniqueness is valued.[27] For the folklorist, these hand-crafted objects embody multifaceted relationships in the lives of the craftsmen and the users, which is completely lacking in mass produced items without connection to an individual craftsman.[28] Regardless of the motivation for the handicraft, this is folklore in action.

Many traditional crafts have been elevated to the fine or applied arts and taught in art schools, such as ironworking and glass-making.[29] Or they are repurposed as folk art, characterized as objects in which the decorative form supersedes its utilitarian needs. Folk art is found in hex signs on Pennsylvania Dutch barns, tin man sculptures made by metalworkers, front yard Christmas displays, decorated school lockers, carved gun stocks, and tattoos. “Words such as naive, self-taught, and individualistic are used to describe these objects, and the exceptional rather than the representative creation is featured.”[30] This is in contrast to our understanding of folklore artifacts which are nurtured and passed along in community.[note 4]

Many objects of material folklore, big and small, are challenging to classify, difficult to archive and unwieldy to store. How do we preserve these bulky artifacts of material culture, and how do we use them? That is the assigned task of museums. Toward this goal the concept of the Living history or open-air museum has been developed, beginning in Scandinavia at the end of the 19th century. These museums are here to teach, not just display. Actors show how items were used, reenacting everyday living by people from all segments of society. In order to achieve this, these museums rely heavily on the material artifacts of a pre-industrial society. Many locations even duplicate the processing of the objects, thus creating new objects of an earlier historic time period. These Living history museums are now found throughout the United States and the world as part of a thriving Heritage industry. This is folklore in action.

This list represents just a small sampling of objects and skills which are included in studies of material culture.

Customs

Customary culture is remembered enactment, i.e. re-enactment. It is the patterns of expected behavior within a group, the “traditional and expected way of doing things”[31] [32] A custom can be a single gesture, such as thumbs down or a handshake. It can also be a complex interaction of multiple folk customs and artifacts as seen in a child’s birthday party, including verbal lore (Happy Birthday song), material lore (presents and a birthday cake), special games (Musical chairs) and individual customs (making a wish as you blow out the candles). Each of these is a folklore artifact in its own right, potentially worthy of investigation and cultural analysis. Together they combine to build the custom of a birthday party celebration, a scripted combination of multiple artifacts which have meaning within their social group.

Santa Claus giving gifts to children, a common folk practice associated with Christmas in Western nations

Folklorists divide customs into several different categories.[33] A custom can be a seasonal celebration, such as Thanksgiving or New Year’s. It can be a life cycle celebration for an individual, such as baptism, birthday or wedding. A custom can also mark a community festival or event; examples of this are Carnival in Cologne or Mardi Gras in New Orleans. This category also includes the Smithsonian Folklife Festival celebrated each summer on the Mall in Washington, DC. A fourth category includes customs related to folk beliefs. Walking under a ladder is just one of many symbols considered unlucky. Occupational groups tend to have a rich history of customs related to their life and work, so the traditions of sailors or lumberjacks.[note 5]The area of ecclesiatical folklore, which includes modes of worship not sanctioned by the established church[34] tends to be so large and complex that it is usually treated as a specialized area of folk customs; it requires considerable expertise in standard church ritual in order to adequately interpret folk customs and beliefs that originated in official church practice.

Customary folklore is by definition folklore in action; it is always a performance, be it a single gesture or a complex of scripted customs. Participating in the custom, either as performer or audience, signifies acknowledgment of that social group. Some customary behavior is intended to be performed and understood only within the group itself, so the handkerchief code sometimes used in the gay community or the initiation rituals of the Freemasons. Other customs are designed specifically to represent a social group to outsiders, those who do not belong to this group. The St. Patrick’s Day Parade in New York and in other communities across the continent is a single example of an ethnic group parading their separateness (differential behavior[35]), and encouraging Americans of all stripes to show alliance to this colorful ethnic group. Another multicolored social group, the Gay Pride Movement, also parades in communities across the country to show the strength of their culture and demonstrate for recognition of their group within the contemporary legal and social systems.

Practitioners of hoodening, a folk custom found in Kent, southeastern England, in 1909

These festivals and parades, with a target audience of people who do not belong to the social group, intersect with the interests and mission of public folklorists, who are engaged in the documentation, preservation, and presentation of traditional forms of folklife. With a swell in popular interest in folk traditions, these community celebrations are becoming more numerous throughout the western world. While ostensibly parading the diversity of their community, economic groups have discovered that these folk parades and festivals are good for business. All shades of people are out on the streets, eating, drinking and spending. This attracts support not only from the business community, but also from federal and state organizations for these local street parties.[36] Paradoxically, in parading diversity within the community, these events have come to authenticate true community, where business interests ally with the varied (folk) social groups to promote the interests of the community as a whole.

This is just a small sampling of types and examples of customary lore.

Childlore and Games

Children's Games (1560) by Pieter Bruegel the Elder, shows five boys playing buck buck in the bottom right hand corner of the painting.

Childlore is a distinct branch of folklore that deals with activities passed on by children to other children, away from the influence or supervision of an adult.[37] Children’s folklore contains artifacts from all the standard folklore genres of verbal, material and customary lore; it is however the child-to-child conduit that distinguishes these artifacts. For childhood is a social group where children teach, learn and share their own traditions, flourishing in a street culture outside the purview of adults. This is also ideally where it needs to be collected; as Iona and Peter Opie demonstrated in their pioneering book Children’s Games in Street and Playground.[38] Here the social group of children is studied on its own terms, not as an derivative of adult social groups. It is shown that the culture of children is quite distinctive; it is generally unnoticed by the sophisticated world of adults, and quite as little affected by it.[39]

Of particular interest to folklorists here is the mode of transmission of these artifacts; this lore circulates exclusively within an informal pre-literate children’s network or folkgroup. It does not include artifacts taught to children by adults. However children can take the taught and teach it further to other children, turning it into childlore. Or they can take the artifacts and turn them into something else; so Old McDonald’s farm is transformed from animal noises to the scatological version of animal poop. This childlore is characterized by “its lack of dependence on literary and fixed form. Children…operate among themselves in a world of informal and oral communication, unimpeded by the necessity of maintaining and transmitting information by written means.[40] This is as close as folklorists can come to observing the transmission and social function of this folk knowledge before the spread of literacy during the 19th century.

As we have seen with the other genres, the original collections of children’s lore and games in the 19th century was driven by a fear that the culture of childhood would die out.[41] Early folklorists, among them Alice Gomme in Britain and William Wells Newell in the United States, felt a need to capture the unstructured and unsupervised street life and activities of children before it was lost. This fear proved to be unfounded. In a comparison of any modern school playground during recess and the painting of “Children’s Games” by Pieter Breugel the Elder we can see that the activity level is similar, and many of the games from the 1560 painting are recognizable and comparable to modern variations still played today.

These same artifacts of childlore, in innumerable variations, also continue to serve the same function of learning and practicing skills needed for growth. So bouncing and swinging rhythms and rhymes encourage development of balance and coordination in infants and children. Verbal rhymes like Peter Piper picked… serve to increase both the oral and aural acuity of children. Songs and chants, accessing a different part of the brain, are used to memorize series (Alphabet song). They also provide the necessary beat to complex physical rhythms and movements, be it hand-clapping, jump roping, or ball bouncing. Furthermore many physical games are used to develop strength, coordination and endurance of the players. For some team games, negotiations about the rules can run on longer than the game itself as social skills are rehearsed.[42] Even as we are just now uncovering the neuroscience that undergirds the developmental function of this childlore, the artifacts themselves have been in play for centuries.

Below is listed just a small sampling of types and examples of childlore and games.

Categories of folklore

Folklore can be classified by ethnicity, region, religion, occupation, and many other categories. Folklore can be a reflection and expression of a group's concerns.[43] Ethnic folklore is a significant indicator of a groups's traditional values.[44]

The "Archie Green Fellows Program" of the Library of Congress honors the legacy of folklorist Archie Green through the Folklife Center’s support of the collecting of occupational folklore. Folklore particular to an occupation will include storytelling, jokes, proverbs, shared beliefs, tools and specialized clothing, and shared knowledge of occupational skills and processes.[45]

National or ethnic

Further information: List of mythologies
For a list of folklores of European countries, see European folklore.

See also

Notes

  1. ^ The word Folkloristics is favored by Alan Dundes, and used in the title of his publication (Dundes 1978). The term Folklore Studies is defined and used by Simon Bronner, see (Bronner 1986, p. xi).
  2. ^ Contributors of this issue were, among others, Claire Farrer, Joan N. Radner, Susan Lanser, Elaine Lawless, and Jeannie B. Thomas.
  3. ^ Vladimir Propp first defined a uniform structure in Russian fairy tales in his groundbreaking monograph Morphology of the Folktale, published in Russian in 1928. See (Propp 1928)
  4. ^ Henry Glassie, a distinguished folklorist studying technology in cultural context, notes that in Turkish one word, sanat, refers to all objects, not distinguishing between art and craft. The latter distinction, Glassie emphasizes, is not based on medium but on social class. This raises the question as to the difference between arts and crafts; is the difference found merely in the labeling?
  5. ^ The folklorist Archie Green specialized in workers’ traditions and the lore of labor groups.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://en.oxforddictionaries.com/definition/lore
  2. ^ (Dundes 1969, p. 13, footnote 34)
  3. ^ (Wilson 2006, p. 85)
  4. ^ (Dundes 1980, p. 7)
  5. ^ (Bauman 1971)
  6. ^ (Dundes 1971)
  7. ^ (Dundes 1965, p. 1)
  8. ^ (Sims & Stephens 2005, pp. 7-8)
  9. ^ (Noyes 2012, p. 20)
  10. ^ (Noyes 2012, pp. 15-16)
  11. ^ (Zumwalt & Dundes 1988)
  12. ^ https://www.loc.gov/folklife/public_law.html
  13. ^ (Hufford 1991)
  14. ^ (Dundes 1980, p. 8)
  15. ^ (Dundes 1980, p. 7)
  16. ^ (Bauman 1971)
  17. ^ (Bauman 1971, p. 41)
  18. ^ (Opie & Opie 1969)
  19. ^ (Georges & Jones 1995, pp. 10-12)
  20. ^ (Toelken 1996, p. 184)
  21. ^ (Sims & Stephens 2005, p. 17)
  22. ^ (Dorson 1972, p. 2)
  23. ^ (Sims & Stephens 2005, p. 13)
  24. ^ (Georges & Jones 1995, pp. 112-113)
  25. ^ (Dorson 1972, p. 2)
  26. ^ (Vlach 1997)
  27. ^ (Roberts 1972, p. 236 ff)
  28. ^ https://muse.jhu.edu/article/33637
  29. ^ (Roberts 1972, p. 236 ff, 250)
  30. ^ http://www.loc.gov/folklife/guide/materialculture.html
  31. ^ (Sweterlitsch 1997, p. 168)
  32. ^ (Sims & Stephens 2005, p. 16)
  33. ^ (Sweterlitsch 1997, p. 168)
  34. ^ (Dorson 1972, p. 4)
  35. ^ (Bauman 1971, p. 45)
  36. ^ (Sweterlitsch 1997, p. 170)
  37. ^ (Grider 1997, p. 123)
  38. ^ (Opie & Opie 1969)
  39. ^ (Grider 1997, p. 125)
  40. ^ (Grider 1997)
  41. ^ (Grider 1997, p. 127)
  42. ^ (Georges & Jones 1995, p. 243 - 254)
  43. ^ Jordan, Rosan A., "Folklore And Ethnicity: Some Theoretical Considerations", Folklife in Louisiana, A Guide to the State, Louisiana Office of Cultural Development, 1985
  44. ^ Stern, Stephen. "Western Folklore", Studies in Folklore and Ethnicity, Vol. 36, No. 1,(Jan., 1977), Western States Folklore Society, DOI: 10.2307/1498212, pp. 7-32
  45. ^ "Folk Art and Material Culture - Occupational Folklore". Retrieved 20 July 2016. 

References

  • Anderson, Walter (1923). "Kaiser und Abt. Die Geschichte eines Schwanks". FF Communications. Helsinki. 42. 
  • Bauman, Richard (1971). "Differential Identity and the Social Base of Folklore". The Journal of American Folklore. 84 (331): 31–41. JSTOR 539731. 
  • Bauman, Richard (2008). "The Philology of the Vernacular". Journal of Folklore Research. 45 (1): 29–36. JSTOR 40206961. 
  • Bauman, Richard; Paredes, Americo, eds. (1972). Toward New Perspectives in Folklore. Bloomington, IN: Trickster Press. p. xv. 
  • Bauman, Richard (1975). "Verbal Art as Performance". American Anthropologist, New Series. Wiley. 77 (2): 290–311. doi:10.1525/aa.1975.77.2.02a00030. JSTOR 674535. 
  • Ben-Amos, Dan (1985). "On the Final [s] in 'Folkloristics'". The Journal of American Folklore. 98 (389): 334–36. JSTOR 539940. 
  • Ben-Amos, Dan (1972). "Toward a Definition of Folklore in Context". In Bauman, Richard; Paredes, Americo. Toward New Perspectives in Folklore. Bloomington, IN: Trickster Press. pp. 3 – 15. 
  • Bendix, Regina (1997). In Search of Authenticity: The Formation of Folklore Studies. Madison: University of Wisconsin Press. 
  • Bendix, Regina; Hasan-Rokem, Galit (eds.). A Companion to Folklore. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. 
  • Blank, Trevor J., ed. (2009). Folklore and the Internet. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press. 
  • Bronner, Simon J. (1986). American Folklore Studies: An Intellectual History. Lawrence, KS: University Press of Kansas. 
  • Bronner, Simon J. (1998). Following Tradition: Folklore in the Discourse of American Culture. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press. ISBN 978-0-87421-239-6. 
  • Bronner, Simon J., ed. (2007). The Meaning of folklore: the Analytical Essays of Alan Dundes. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press. 
  • Bronner, Simon J., ed. (1985). American Material Culture and Folklife: A Prologue and Dialogue. Ann Arbor, MI: UMI Research Press. 
  • Brunvand, Jan Harald (1968). The Study of American Folklore. New York, London: W.W. Norton. 
  • Burns, Thomas A. (1977). "Folkloristics: A Conception of Theory". Western Folklore. 36 (2): 109–134. JSTOR 1498964. 
  • Cicala, John (2009). "Pathfinder: American Folklife Resources". 
  • Del-Rio-Roberts, Maribel (2010). "A Guide to Conducting Ethnographic Research: A Review of Ethnography: Step-by-Step (3rd ed.) by David M. Fetterman" (PDF). The Qualitative Report. 15 (3): 737–749. 
  • Deloria, Vine (1994). God Is Red: A Native View of Religion. Golden, CO: Fulcrum Publishing. 
  • Dorson, Richard (1976). Folklore and Fakelore: Essays Toward a Discipline of Folk Studies. Cambridge, London: Harvard University Press. ISBN 9780674330207. 
  • Dorson, Richard, ed. (1972). Folklore and Folklife: an Introduction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 
  • Dorst, John (2016). "Folklore's Cybernetic Imaginary, or, Unpacking the Obvious". Journal of American Folklore. American Folklore Society. 129 (512): 127–145. JSTOR 10.5406/jamerfolk.129.512.0127. 
  • Dorst, John (1990). "Tags and Burners, Cycles and Networks: Folklore in the Telectronic Age". Journal of Folklore Research. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. 27 (3): 61–108. 
  • Dundes, Alan (1969). "The Devolutionary Premise in Folklore Theory". Journal of the Folklore Institute. Indiana University Press. 6 (1): 5–19. doi:10.2307/3814118. JSTOR 3814118. 
  • Dundes, Alan (1978). Essays in Folkloristics (Kirpa Dai series in folklore and anthropology). Folklore Institute. 
  • Dundes, Alan (1971). "Folk Ideas as Units of Worldview". The Journal of American Folklore. 84 (331): 93–103. doi:10.2307/539737. JSTOR 539737. 
  • Dundes, Alan (2005). "Folkloristics in the Twenty-First Century (AFS Invited Presidential Plenary Address, 2004)". Journal of Folklore. 118 (470): 385–408. JSTOR 4137664. 
  • Dundes, Alan (1980). Interpreting Folklore. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. 
  • Dundes, Alan (1978). "Into the Endzone for a Touchdown: A Psychoanalytic Consideration of American Football". Western Folklore. 37 (2): 75–88. JSTOR 1499315. 
  • Dundes, Alan (1984). Life Is like a Chicken Coop Ladder. A Portrait of German Culture through Folklore. New York: Columbia University Press. 
  • Dundes, Alan (1965). The Study of Folklore. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. 
  • Bauman, Richard; Paredes, Americo, eds. (1972). Toward New Perspectives in Folklore. Bloomington, IN: Trickster Press. 
  • Ellis, Bill (2002). "Making a Big Apple Crumble". New Directions in Folklore (6). 
  • Fixico, Donald L. (2003). The American Indian Mind in a Linear World. New York: Routledge. 
  • Frank, Russel (2009). "The Forward as Folklore: Studying E-Mailed Humor". In Blank, Trevor J. Folklore and the Internet. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press. pp. 98–122. 
  • Gazin-Schwartz, Amy (2011). "Myth and Folklore". In Insoll, Timothy. The Oxford Handbook of the Archaeology of Ritual and Religion. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 63–75. ISBN 978-0199232444. 
  • Genzuk, Michael (2003). "A Synthesis of Ethnographic Research". Center for Multilingual, Multicultural Research. University of Southern California. 
  • Georges, Robert A.; Jones, Michael Owen (1995). Folkloristics : an Introduction. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. ISBN 978-0253209948. 
  • Glassie, Henry (1983). "The Moral Lore of Folklore" (PDF). Folklore Forum. 16 (2): 123–151. 
  • Goody, Jack (1977). The Domestication of the Savage Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 
  • Grider, Sylvia (1997). "Children's Folklore". In Green, Thomas. Folklore An Encyclopedia of Beliefs, Customs, Tales, Music, and Art. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO. pp. 123 – 128. 
  • Hufford, Mary (1991). "American Folklife: A Commonwealth of Cultures". Publication of the American Folklife Center. 17: 1–23. 
  • Kirshenblatt-Gimblett, Barbara (1985). "Di folkloristik: A Good Yiddish Word". The Journal of American Folklore. 98 (389): 331–334. JSTOR 539939. 
  • Kirshenblatt-Gimblett, Barbara (September 1999). "Performance Studies". Rockefeller Foundation, Culture and Creativity. 
  • Mason, Bruce Lionel (1998). "E-Texts: The Orality and Literacy Issue Revisited". Oral Traditions. 13. Columbia, MO: Center for Studies in Oral Tradition. 
  • Merton, Ambrose (1965). "What is Folklore?". In Dundes, Alan. The Study of Folklore. Berkeley, CA: University of California. pp. 1 – 6. 
  • Noyes, Dorothy (2003). "Group". In Feintuch, Burt. Eight Words for the Study of Expressive Culture. University of Illinois Press. pp. 7–41. JSTOR 10.5406/j.ctt2ttc8f.5. 
  • Noyes, Dorothy (2012). "The Social Base of Folklore". In Bendix, Regina; Hasan-Rokem, Galit. A Companion to Folklore. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 13–39. 
  • Opie, Iona; Opie, Peter (1969). Children's Games in Street and Playground. Oxford University Press. 
  • Oring, Elliott (1986). Folk Groups and Folklore Genres. An Introduction. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press. 
  • Propp, Vladimir (1928). Morphology of the Folktale. Leningrad. 
  • Raskin, Victor, ed. (2008). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 
  • Roberts, Warren (1972). "Folk Crafts". In Dorson, Richard. Folklore and Folklife: an Introduction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pp. 233–252. 
  • Roberts, Warren E. (1990). Viewpoints on Folklife: Looking at the Overlooked. Ann Arbor, MI: UMI Research Press. 
  • Schmidt-Lauber, Brigitta (2012). Bendix, Regina; Hasan-Rokem, Galit, eds. "Seeing, Hearing, Feeling, Writing: Approaches and Methods in Ethnographic Research from the Perspective of Ethnological Analyses of the Present". A companion to Folklore Studies. Oxford Wiley Blackwell: 559–578. 
  • Sims, Martha; Stephens, Martine (2005). Living Folklore: Introduction to the Study of People and their Traditions. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press. 
  • Šmidchens, Guntis (1999). "Folklorism Revisited". Journal of American Folklore Research. 36 (1): 51–70. JSTOR 3814813. 
  • Stahl, Sandra D. (1989). Literary Folkloristics and the Personal Narrative. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. ISBN 978-0915305483. 
  • Sweterlitsch, Richard (1997). "Custom". In Green, Thomas. Folklore An Encyclopedia of Beliefs, Customs, Tales, Music, and Art. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO. pp. 168–172. 
  • Toelken, Barre (1996). The Dynamics of Folklore. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press. 
  • Vlach, John (1997). "Material Culture". In Green, Thomas. Folklore An Encyclopedia of Beliefs, Customs, Tales, Music, and Art. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO. pp. 540–544. 
  • Wilson, William (2006). Rudy, Jill Terry; Call, Diane, eds. The Marrow of Human Experience: Essays on Folklore. University Press of Colorado. JSTOR j.ctt4cgkmk. 
  • Wolf-Knuts, Ulrika (1999). "On the history of comparison in folklore studies". Folklore Fellows' Summer School. 
  • Yoder, Don, ed. (1976). American Folklife. Austin, TX: University of Texas. 
  • Yoder, Don (2001). Discovering American Folklife: Essays on Folk Culture & the Pennsylvania Dutch. Mechanicsburg, PA: Stackpole Books. 
  • Zumwalt, Rosemary Levy; Dundes, Alan (1988). American Folklore Scholarship: A Dialogue of Dissent. Indiana University Press. 

Further reading